Volume 13 · Number 1 · Pages 59–67
Varela’s Radical Proposal: How to Embody and Open Up Cognitive Science

Kristian Moltke Martiny

Download the full text in
PDF (901 kB)

> Citation > Similar > References > Add Comment

Abstract

Context: The scientific landscape of cognitive science is today influenced, as are other areas of science, by the open science movement, which is seen, for instance, in the recently launched Open MIND project. Problem: More than 25 years ago Varela introduced the idea of opening up cognitive science. He called for a radical transformation of values, training and ways to conduct cognitive science. Yet, his radical proposal has been neglected in the discussions in cognitive science. Method: I describe Varela’s proposal by revisiting his philosophical arguments, his embodied and enactive view of cognition, and the methods he proposed as an alternative, namely the neurophenomenological and the second-person method. Results: I show how cognitive scientists neglect Varela’s proposal, because as scientists we are part of a scientific tradition and community that has not developed a research practice that enables us to integrate his proposal. I discuss different attempts to integrate the proposal into the research practice of cognitive science using the phenomenological interview, and argue for an even more radical approach. Implications: If we, as cognitive scientists, do not develop “how” we do cognitive science and change the scientific community we are embedded in, we will not be able to open up cognitive science and fully address the experiential, embodied and enactive aspects of cognition. Varela’s radical proposal for how to do so is therefore as important today as ever.

Key words: Cognitive science, embodying the mind, enaction, phenomenological interview, intersubjective validation

Citation

Martiny K. M. (2017) Varela’s radical proposal: How to embody and open up cognitive science. Constructivist Foundations 13(1): 59–67. http://constructivist.info/13/1/059

Export article citation data: Plain Text · BibTex · EndNote · Reference Manager (RIS)

Similar articles

Vörös S., Froese T. & Riegler A. (2016) Epistemological Odyssey: Introduction to Special Issue on the Diversity of Enactivism and Neurophenomenology
Vörös S. (2014) The Uroboros of Consciousness: Between the Naturalisation of Phenomenology and the Phenomenologisation of Nature
Vörös S. & Riegler A. (2017) A Plea for not Watering Down the Unseemly: Reconsidering Francisco Varela’s Contribution to Science
Vörös S. & Bitbol M. (2017) Enacting Enaction: A Dialectic Between Knowing and Being
Froese T., Gould C. & Barrett A. (2011) Re-Viewing from Within: A Commentary on First- and Second-Person Methods in the Science of Consciousness

References

Becchio C., Sartori L. & Castiello U. (2010) Toward you: The social side of actions. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19(3): 183–188. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Berkovich-Ohana A. (2017) Radical neurophenomenology: We cannot solve the problems using the same kind of thinking we used when we created them. Constructivist Foundations 12(2): 156–159. http://constructivist.info/12/2/156
Bitbol M. & Antonova A. (2016) On the too often overlooked radicality of neurophenomenology. Constructivist Foundations 11(2): 354–356. http://constructivist.info/11/2/354
Brinkmann S. & Kvale S. (2014) Interviews: Learning the craft of qualitative research interviewing. Third edition. SAGE, Los Angeles.. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
de Bruin L., Van Elk M. & Newen A. (2012) Reconceptualizing second-person interaction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6: 151. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
de Jaegher H., Di Paolo E. & Gallagher S. (2010) Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in cognitive sciences 14(10): 441–447. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Depraz N., Varela F. J. & Vermersch P. (eds.) (2003) On becoming aware. John Benjamins, Amsterdam. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Desbordes G. & Negi L. (2013) A new era for mind studies: Training investigators in both scientific and contemplative methods of inquiry. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7: 741. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Dewey J. (1922) Human nature and conduct: An introduction to social psychology. G. Allen & Unwin, London. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Dorjee D. (2016) Defining contemplative science: The metacognitive self-regulatory capacity of the mind, context of meditation practice and modes of existential awareness. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1788. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Flyvbjerg B. (2011) Case study. In: Denzin N. K. & Lincoln Y. S. (eds.) The Sage handbook of qualitative research. Fourth edition. Sage, Thousand Oaks: 301–316. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Gallagher S. & Marcel A. (1999) The self in contextualized action. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6(4): 4–30. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Goldman A. & de Vignemont F. (2009) Is social cognition embodied? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(4): 154–159. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Husserl E. (1970) The crisis of European sciences and transcendental phenomenology. Translated by David Carr. Northwestern University Press, Evanston IL. German original published in 1936. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Husserl E. (1973) Husserliana XIII: Zur Phänomenologie der Intersubjektivität. Texte aus dem Nachlass. Erster Teil: 1905–1920. Edited by I. Kern. Martinus Nijhoff, Den Haag. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Høffding S. & Martiny K. (2016) Framing a phenomenological interview: What, why and how. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 15(4): 539–564. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Klimecki O. M., Leiberg S., Ricard M. & Singer T. (2014) Differential pattern of functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9(6):873–879. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Kraker P., Leony D., Reinhardt W. & Beham G. (2011) The case for an open science in technology enhanced learning. International Journal of Technology Enhanced Learning 3(6): 643–654. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Lachaux J.-P. (2011) If no control, then what? Making sense of “neural noise” in human brain mapping experiments using first person reports. Journal of Consciousness Studies 18: 162–166. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Le Van Quyen M. & Petitmengin C. (2002) Neuronal dynamics and conscious experience: An example of reciprocal causation before epileptic seizures. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1: 169–180. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Leiter K. (1980) A primer on ethnomethodology. Oxford University Press, Oxford. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Levenson R. W., Ekman P. & Ricard M. (2012) Meditation and the startle response: A case study. Emotion 12: 650–658. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Lutz A. (2002) Toward a neurophenomenology as an account of generative passages: A first empirical case study. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1(2): 133–167. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Lutz A., Brefczynski-Lewis J., Johnstone T. & Davidson R. J. (2008) Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: Effects of meditative expertise. PLoS ONE 3: e1897. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Lutz A., Greischar L. L., Rawlings N. B., Ricard M. & Davidson R. J. (2004) Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101: 16369–16373. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Lutz A., Lachaux J.-P., Martinerie J. & Varela F. J. (2002) Guiding the study of brain dynamics by using first-person data: Synchrony patterns correlate with ongoing conscious states during a simple visual task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 99(3): 1586–1591. http://cepa.info/2092
Martiny K. M. & Aggerholm K. (2016) Embodying cognition: Working with self-control in cerebral palsy. The Cognitive Behaviour Therapist 9: e33. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Martiny K. M. M. (2015) Embodying investigations of cerebral palsy: A case of open cognitive science. Doctoral dissertation, University of Copenhagen, Faculty of Humanities, Department of Media, Cognition and Communication. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Martiny K. M., Pedersen D. B. & Birkegaard A. (2016) Open media science. Journal of science communication 15(6): A02–2. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Merleau-Ponty M. (1945) Phénoménologie de la perception. Librairie Gallimard, Paris. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Metzinger T. K. & Windt J. M. (2015) What does it mean to have an Open MIND? In: Metzinger T. K. & Windt J. M. (eds.) Open MIND. MIND Group, Frankfurt am Main: 0(C). https://open-mind.net/papers/general-introduction-what-does-it-mean-to-have-an-open-mind
Neisser U. (1980) On social knowing. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 6(4): 601–605. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Nisbett R. E. & Wilson T. D. (1977) Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84(3): 231–259. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Petitmengin C. & Bitbol M. (2009) Listening from within. Journal of Consciousness Studies 16(10–12): 363–404. http://cepa.info/2373
Petitmengin C. (2006) Describing one’s subjective experience in the second person: An interview method for the science of consciousness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 5: 229–269. http://cepa.info/2376
Petitmengin C. (2017) Enaction as a lived experience: Towards a radical neurophenomenology. Constructivist Foundations 12(2): 139–147. http://constructivist.info/12/2/139
Petitmengin C., Navarro V. & Baulac M. (2006) Seizure anticipation: Are neuro-phenomenological approaches able to detect preictal symptoms? Epilepsy and Behavior 9: 298–306. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Petitmengin C., Navarro V. & Le Van Quyen M. (2007) Anticipating seizure: Pre-reflective experience at the center of neuro-phenomenology. Consciousness and Cognition 16: 746–764. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Petitmengin C., Remillieux A., Cahour B., Carter-Thomas S. (2013) A gap in Nisbett and Wilson’s findings? A first-person access to our cognitive processes. Consciousness and Cognition 22(2): 654–669. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Petitmengin C., Van Beek M, Bitbol M., Nissou J-M. & Roepstorff A. (2017) What is it like to meditate? Methods and issues for a micro-phenomenological description of meditative experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies 24(5–6): 170–198. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Reinerman-Jones L., Sollins B., Gallagher S. & Janz B. (2013) Neurophenomenology: An integrated approach to exploring awe and wonder. South African Journal of Philosophy 32(4): 295–309. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Remillieux A., Petitmengin C., Ermine J-L., Blatter C. (2010) Knowledge sharing in change management: A case study in the French railways company. Journal of Knowledge Management Practice 11(2). ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Roschelle J. (1998) Choosing and using video equipment for data collection. In: Kelly A. & Lesh R. (eds.) Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education. Lawrence Erlbaum, London: PAGES. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Rowlands M. (2010) The new science of the mind: From extended mind to embodied phenomenology. MIT Press, Cambridge MA. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Satne G. & Roepstorff A. (2015) Introduction: From interacting agents to engaging persons. Journal of Consciousness Studies 22(1–2): 9–23. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Schilbach L., Timmermans B., Reddy V., Costall A., Bente G., Schlicht T. & Vogeley K. (2013) Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(4): 393–414. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Schön D. A. (1987) Educating the reflective practitioner: Toward a new design for teaching and learning in the professions. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco. Republished in 2002. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Stewart J., Gapenne O. & Di Paolo E. A. (2010) Enaction: Toward a new paradigm for cognitive science. MIT Press, Cambridge. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Suchman L. A. & Trigg R. H. (1995) Understanding practice: Video as a medium for reflection and design (excerpt). In: Baecker R. M., Grudin J., Buxton W. A. S. & Greenberg S. (eds.) Human-computer interaction: Toward the year 2000. Morgan Kaufmann, San Francisco: 233–240. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Thompson E. (ed.) (2001) Between ourselves: Second-person issues in the study of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8(5–7). ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Valenzuela-Moguillansky C., O’Regan J. K. & Petitmengin C. (2013) Exploring the subjective experience of the “rubber hand” illusion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7:659. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Varela F. J. & Shear J. (1999) First-person methodologies: What, why, how? Journal of Consciousness Studies 6(2–3): 1–14. http://cepa.info/2080
Varela F. J. (1996) Neurophenomenology: A methodological remedy for the hard problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies 3(4): 330–349. http://cepa.info/1893
Varela F. J. (1999) Ethical know-how: Action, wisdom, and cognition. Stanford University Press, Stanford CA. http://cepa.info/2119
Varela F. J., Thompson E. & Rosch E. (1991) The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press, Cambridge MA. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Vermersch P. (1994) L’entretien d’explicitation. ESF Éditeur, Paris. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Vermersch P. (1999) Introspection as practice. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6(2–3): 17–42. http://cepa.info/2513
Vermersch P. (2009) Describing the practice of introspection. Journal of Consciousness Studies 16(10–12): 20–57. http://cepa.info/2416
Ward D. & Stapleton M. (2012) Es are good: Cognition as enacted, embodied, embedded, affective and extended. In: Paglieri F. (ed.) Consciousness in interaction: The role of the natural and social context in shaping consciousness. John Benjamins, Amsterdam: 89–104. http://cepa.info/2292
Watson M. (2015) When will “open science” become simply “science”? Genome Biology 16: 101. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Zahavi D. (1999) Self-awareness and alterity: A phenomenological investigation. Northwestern University Press, Evanston IL. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Zahavi D. (2005) Subjectivity and selfhood: Investigating the first-person perspective. MIT Press, Cambridge MA. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Zahavi D. (2011) Varieties of reflection. Journal of Consciousness Studies 18(2): 9–19. ▸︎ Google︎ Scholar

Comments: 0

To stay informed about comments to this publication and post comments yourself, please log in first.