Perception-Action Mutuality Obviates Mental Construction
Martin Flament-Fultot, Lin Nie & Claudia Carello
Log in to download the full text for free
> Citation
> Similar
> References
> Add Comment
Abstract
Context: The dominant approach to the study of perception is representational/computational, with an emphasis on the achievements of the brain and the nervous system, which are taken to construct internal models of the world. Alternatives include ecological, embedded, embodied, and enactivist approaches, all of which emphasize the centrality of action in understanding perception. Problem: Despite sharing many theoretical commitments that lead to a rejection of the classical approach, the alternatives are characterized by important contrasts and points of divergence. Here we focus on the enactive and ecological approaches, in particular, on how they construe the status of the environment and the content of perception. Method: We begin with a review of James Gibson’s ecological psychology, highlighting it as a psychology for all organisms not just humans. Against this backdrop, we consider enactivist arguments against direct perception - a central assertion of the ecological approach - and in favor of interpreting the activity of perceptual agents as a kind of construction of a perceptually meaningful world. Results: We assess the merits of this interpretation and we conclude that it cannot be grounded on fundamental principles such as thermodynamics and organism-environment mutuality. Implications: As a consequence, enactivism remains close to representationalism and entails a form of dualism. Constructivist content: We advance a criticism of the constructivist foundations of the enactive approach to perception. Perception-action mutuality at the heart of enactivism does not require mental construction; indeed, perception-action mutuality obviates construction.
Key words: Ecological psychology, Gibson, constructivism, enactivism, perception, representation.
Citation
Flament-Fultot M., Nie L. & Carello C. (2016) Perception-action mutuality obviates mental construction. Constructivist Foundations 11(2): 298–307. http://constructivist.info/11/2/298
Export article citation data:
Plain Text ·
BibTex ·
EndNote ·
Reference Manager (RIS)
Similar articles
References
Annila A. & Salthe S. (2010) Physical foundations of evolutionary theory. Journal of Non-Equilibrium Thermodynamics 35: 301–321.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Beer R. (2003) The dynamics of active categorical perception in an evolved agent. Adaptive Behavior 11: 209–244.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Bennett M. R. & Hacker M. (2003) Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. Blackwell Publishing, Oxford UK.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Calvo P. & Keijzer F. (2011) Plants: Adaptive behavior, root brains and minimal cognition. Adaptive Behavior 19: 155–171.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Calvo P., Raja V. & Lee D. N. (2015) Guidance of circumnutation of climbing bean stems: An ecological exploration. Technical Report #15–11(1) Minimal Intelligence Lab (MINT Lab), Universidad de Murcia, Spain.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Cappuccio M. & Froese T. (2014) Introduction. In: Cappuccio M. & Froese T. (eds.) Enactive cognition at the edge of sense-making: Making sense of non-sense. Palgrave Macmillan, Basingstoke UK: 1–33.
http://cepa.info/2478
Carello C. (2004) Perceiving affordances by dynamic touch: Hints from the control of movement. Ecological Psychology 16: 31–36.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Cariani P. (2011) The semiotics of cybernetic percept-action systems. International Journal of Signs and Semiotic Systems 1(1): 1–17.
http://cepa.info/2534
Cariani P. (2015) Sign functions in natural and artificial systems. In: Trifonas P. P. (ed.) International handbook of semiotics. Springer, Dordrecht: 917–950.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Chemero A. (2001) What we perceive when we perceive affordances: Commentary on Michaels (2000) “Information, perception, and action.” Ecological Psychology 13(2): 111–116.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Chomsky N. (1980) Rules and representations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3: 1–15.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Clark A. (2016) Surfing uncertainty. Prediction, action and the embodied mind. Oxford University Press, New York.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Costantini M. & Stapleton M. (2016) How the body narrows the interaction with the environment. In: Coello Y. & Fischer M. H. (eds.) Perceptual and emotional embodiment: Foundations of embodied cognition. Volume 1. Routledge, New York: 181–198.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Davidson D. (1970) Mental events. In: Foster L. & Swanson J. W. (eds.) Experience and theory. Duckworth, London: 79–101. Reprinted in: Davidson D. (1980) Actions and events. Clarendon, Oxford: 207–225.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Davidson D. (1987) Knowing one’s own mind. Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association 60(3): 441–458.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Davis T. J., Kay B. A., Kondepudi D. & Dixon J. A. (2016) Spontaneous interentity coordination in a dissipative structure. Ecological Psychology 28: 23–36.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Degenaar J. (2013) Through the inverting glass: First-person observations on spatial vision and imagery. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 13(2): 373–393.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Di Paolo E. (2016) The enactive conception of life. In: Newen A., Gallagher S. & de Bruin L. (eds.) Oxford handbook of embodied, embedded, extended, enactive cognitive science. Oxford University Press, Cambridge UK. In: Press.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Di Paolo E. A. (2009) Overcoming autopoiesis: An enactive detour on the way from life to society. In: Magalhaes R. & Sanchez R. (eds.) Autopoiesis in organizations and information systems. Emerald, Bingley: 43–68.
http://cepa.info/2366
Dicke M., van Loon J. J. & Soler R. (2009) Chemical complexity of volatiles from plants induced by multiple attack. Nature Chemical Biology,5(5): 317–324.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Dotov D. G. & Chemero A. (2014) Experimental phenomenology: Implications for theories of perception and movement science. In: Cappuccio M. & Froese T. (eds.) Enactive cognition at the edge of sense-making: Making sense of non-sense. Palgrave Macmillan, Basingstoke UK: 37–60.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Fodor J. A. & Pylyshyn Z. W. (1981) How direct is visual perception? Some reflections on Gibson’s “ecological approach.” Cognition 9(2): 139–196.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Frápolli M. J. & Villanueva N. (2012) Minimal expressivism. Dialectica 66(4): 471–487.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Froese T. (2011) From second-order cybernetics to enactive cognitive science: Varela’s turn from epistemology to phenomenology. Systems Research and Behavioral Science 28: 631–645.
http://cepa.info/449
Froese T. (2014) Review of : Radicalizing Enactivism: Basic Minds without Content. Daniel D. Hutto and Erik Myin. The Journal of Mind and Behavior 35(1–2): 71–82.
http://cepa.info/2273
Føllesdal D. (1969) Husserl’s notion of noema. Journal of Philosophy 66: 680–687.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Gibson J. J. (1972) A direct theory of visual perception. In: Royce J. & Rozeboom W. W. (eds.) The psychology of knowing. Gordon and Breach, New York.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Gibson J. J. (1977) The theory of affordances. In: Shaw R. & Bransford J. (eds.) Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology. Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ: 67–82.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Gibson J. J. (1982) The affordances of the environment. In: Reed E. & Jones R. (eds.) Reasons for realism: Selected essays of James J. Gibson. Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ: 408–410.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Godfrey-Smith P. (1996) Complexity and the function of mind in nature. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge UK.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Gurwitsch A. (1974) Perceptual coherence as the foundation of the judgment of predication. In: Gurwitsch A. (eds.) Phenomenology and the theory of science. Northwestern University Press, Evanston IL: 241–267.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Heft H. (1989) Affordances and the body: An intentional analysis of Gibson’s ecological approach to visual perception. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 19(1): 1–30.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Heft H. (2001) Ecological psychology in context: James Gibson, Roger Barker, and the legacy of William James’s radical empiricism. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah NJ.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Heft H. (2007) The social constitution of perceiver-environment reciprocity. Ecological Psychology 19(2): 85–105.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Heidegger M. (1962) Being and time. Harper and Raw, New York. German original published in 1927.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Husserl E. (2014) Ideas for a pure phenomenology and phenomenological philosophy. Hackett, Indianapolis.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Iberall A. S. (1974) Bridges in science – from physics to social science. General Technical Services, Inc., Upper Darby PA.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Jaeger H. (1998) Today’s dynamical systems are too simple. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(5): 643–644.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
James W. (1905) How two minds can know one thing. The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods 2: 176–181.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Johnston M. (2011) Saving God. Religion after idolatry. Princeton University Press, Princeton.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Kelso J. A. S. (2009) Synergies: Atoms of brain and behavior. In: Sternad D. (ed.) Progress in motor control. Springer, New York: 83–91.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Klawiter A. (2012) Co ze mną zrobisz kiedy mnie zobaczysz. Percepcja jako wyszukiwanie ofert (affordances) w otoczeniu [What would you do if you saw me. Perception as seeking for affordances]. Avant 3(2): 261–266.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Kleidon A., Malhi Y. & Cox P. M. (eds.) (2010) Maximum entropy production in ecological and environmental systems: Applications and implications. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 365(1545).
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Kondepudi D. K., Kay B. A. & Dixon J. A. (2015) End-directed evolution and the emergence of energy-seeking behavior in a complex system. Physical Review E 91: 050902–1–050902–5.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Kugler P. N., Kelso J. A. S. & Turvey M. T. (1980) On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures: I. Theoretical lines of convergence. In: Stelmach G. E. & Requin J. (eds.) Tutorials in motor behavior. North Holland, Amsterdam: 1–47.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Lewontin R. C. (1983) The organism as the subject and object of evolution. Scientia 118: 65–95.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Lopresti-Goodman S., Turvey M. T. & Frank T. (2011) Behavioral dynamics of the affordance “graspable.” Attention, Perception & Psychophysics 73: 1948–1965.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Mace W. M. (1977) James J. Gibson’s strategy for perceiving: Ask not what’s inside your head but what your head’s inside of. In: Shaw R. & Bransford J. (eds.) Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology. Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ: 43–65.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Malone L. J. & Dolter T. (2008) Basic concepts of chemistry. 8th Edition. John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken NJ.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Martin C. B. (2008) The mind in nature. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
McDowell J. (1994) The content of perceptual experience. The Philosophical Quarterly 44(175): 190–205.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Michaelian K. (2012) Biological catalysis of the hydrological cycle: Life’s thermodynamic function. Hydrology and Earth Systems Science 16: 2629–2645.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Michaels & Palatinus (2014) A ten commandments for ecological psychology. In: Shapiro L. (ed.) The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition. Routledge, Abingdon: 19–28.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Michaels C. F. & Palatinus Z. (2014) A ten commandments for ecological psychology. In: Shapiro L. (ed.) The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition. Routledge, New York: 19–28.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Michaels C. F. (2003) Affordances: Four points of debate. Ecological Psychology 15: 135–148.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Molnar G. (2003) Powers. A study in metaphysics. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Natsoulas T. (1994) An introduction to reflexive seeing. Journal of Mind and Behavior 1(3): 351–374.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Oyama S. (2000) The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution. Duke University Press, Durham.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Pinedo M. & Noble J. (2007) Beyond persons: Extending the personal/subpersonal distinction to non-rational animals and artificial agents. Biology & Philosophy 23: 87–100.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Reed E. S. & Jones R. (1982) Reasons for realism: Selected essays of James J. Gibson. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale NJ.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Reed E. S. (1988) James J. Gibson and the psychology of perception. Yale University Press, New Haven CT.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Reed E. S. (1993) The intention to use a specific affordance: A framework for psychology. In: Wozniak R. & Fisscher K. (eds.) Development in context: Acting and thinking in specific environments. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ: 45–75.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Reed E. S. (1996) Encountering the world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Richardson M. J., Marsh K. L. & Baron R. M. (2007) Judging and actualizing intrapersonal and interpersonal affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology 33: 845–859.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Rietveld E. (2008) Special section: The skillful body as a concernful system of possible actions: Phenomena and neurodynamics. Theory & Psychology 18: 341–363.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Rietveld E. (2012) Context-switching and responsiveness to real relevance. In: Kiverstein J. & Wheeler M. (eds.) Heidegger and cognitive science. Palgrave Macmillan, Basingstoke UK: 105–135.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Rochat P., Goubetb N. & Senders S. J. (1999) To reach or not to reach? Perception of body effectivities by young infants. Infant and Child Development 8: 129–148.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Runeson S. (1977) On the possibility of “smart” perceptual mechanisms. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 18: 172–179.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Scarinzi A. (2011) Understanding skill acquisition: The enactive vs. the ecological approach and some consequences. BIO Web of Conferences 1: 00080.
http://cepa.info/2412
Sellars W. (1956) Empiricism and the philosophy of mind. In: Feigl H. & Scriven M. (eds.) Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science. Volume 1. University of Minnesota Press, Mineapolis: 253–329.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Shaw R. E. & Kinsella-Shaw J. M. (1988) Ecological mechanics: A physical geometry for intentional constraints. Human Movement Sciences 7(2): 155–200.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Shaw R. E. & McIntyre M. (1974) Algoristic foundations to cognitive psychology. In: Weimer W. & Palermo D. (eds.) Cognition and the symbolic processes. Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ: 305–362.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Shaw R. E. & Turvey M. T. (1999) Ecological foundations of cognition II: Degrees of freedom and conserved quantities in animal-environment systems. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6(11–12): 111–124.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Shaw R. E. (2001) Processes, acts, and experiences: Three stances on the problem of intentionality. Ecological Psychology 13: 275–314.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Shaw R. E., Turvey M. T. & Mace W. M. (1982) Ecological psychology: The consequence of a commitment to realism. In: Weimer W. & Palermo D. (eds.) Cognition and the symbolic processes II. Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ: 159–226.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Stapleton M. & Froese T. (2015) Is collective agency a coherent idea? Considerations from the enactive theory of agency. In: Misselhorn C. (ed.) Collective agency and cooperation in natural and artificial systems. Springer, Cham: 219–236.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Stapleton M. & Froese T. (2016) The enactivist philosophy of embodiment: From biological foundations of agency to the phenomenology of subjectivity. In: García-Valdecasas M., Murillo J. I. & Barrett N. F. (eds.) Biology and subjectivity: Philosophical contributions to a non-reductive neuroscience. Springer, Dordrecht. In: Press.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Swenson R. (1989) Emergent attractors and the law of maximum entropy production: Foundations to a theory of general evolution. Systems Research 6: 187–197.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Swenson R. (1992) Autocatakinetics, yes – autopoiesis, no: Steps toward a unified theory of evolutionary ordering. International Journal of General Systems 21: 207–228.
http://cepa.info/2536
Swenson R. (2009) The fourth law of thermodynamics or the law of maximum entropy production (LMEP). Chemistry 18: 333–339.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Thompson E. (2004) Life and mind: From autopoiesis to neurophenomenology. A tribute to Francisco Varela. Phenomenological Cognitive Science 3: 381–398.
http://cepa.info/1137
Thompson E. (2005) Sensorimotor subjectivity and the enactive approach to experience. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 4(4): 407–427.
http://cepa.info/2358
Tschacher W. & Dauwalder J.–P. (1999) Situated cognition, ecological perception, and synergetics: A novel perspective for cognitive psychology? In: Tschacher W. & Dauwalder J.–P. (eds.) Dynamics, synergetics autonomous agents. World Scientific, Singapore: 83–104.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Turvey M. T. & Fonseca S. T. (2014) The medium of haptic perception: A tensegrity hypothesis. Journal of motor behavior 46(3): 143–187.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Turvey M. T. & Shaw R. E. (1979) The primacy of perceiving: An ecological reformulation of perception for understanding memory. In: Nilssen L. G. (ed.) In perspectives on memory research: Essays in honor of Uppsala University’s 500th anniversary. Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ: 167–222.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Turvey M. T. & Shaw R. E. (1995) Toward an ecological physics and a physical psychology. In: Solso R. & Massaro D. (eds.) The science of the mind: 2001 and beyond. Oxford University Press, Oxford: 144–169.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Turvey M. T. (2013) Ecological perspective on perception-action: What kind of science does it entail? In: Prinz W., Bessard M. & Herwig A. (eds.) Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline. MIT Press, Cambridge MA: 139–170.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Turvey M. T. (2015) Quantum-like issues at nature’s ecological scale (the scale of organisms and their environments). Mind and Matter 13(1): 7–44.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Turvey M. T., Shaw R. E. & Mace W. (1978) Issues in the theory of action: Degrees of freedom, coordinative structures and coalitions. In: Requin J. (ed.) Attention and performance VII. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ: 557–595.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Wagman J. & Carello C. (2003) Haptically creating affordances: Grasping so as to create functional objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 9: 175–186.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Warren W. H. (1988) Action modes and laws of control for the visual guidance of action. In: Meijer O. & Roth K. (eds.) Movement behavior: The motor–action controversy. North-Holland, Amsterdam: 339–380.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Werner K. (2016) Husserl o aspektowości percepcji [Husserl on the aspectual shape of perception]. Diametros. In: Press.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Withagen R., de Poel H. J., Araújo D. & Pepping G. J. (2012) Affordances can invite behavior: Reconsidering the relationship between affordances and agency. New Ideas in Psychology 30(2): 250–258.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Wittgenstein L. (1953) Philosophical investigations. Translated by G. E. M. Anscombe. Macmillan, New York.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Zukow-Goldring P. & Arbib M. A. (2007) Affordances, effectivities, and assisted imitation: Caregivers and the directing of attention. Neurocomputing 70: 2181–2193.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Comments: 0
To stay informed about comments to this publication and post comments yourself, please log in first.