Subjective Experiences are Relational: Implications for Trauma Research and Therapy
Anna Ciaunica
Log in to download the full text for free
> Citation
> Similar
> References
> Add Comment
Abstract
Open peer commentary on the article “Applying the Neurophenomenological Approach to the Study of Trauma: Theory and Practice Yochai Ataria, Mooli Lahad & Omer Horovitz The Study of Trauma” by Yochai Ataria, Mooli Lahad & Omer Horovitz. Abstract: While I applaud Ataria and colleagues’ emphasis on the importance of “returning” to the phenomenology of disrupted subjective experiences, here I draw attention to the importance of second-person body-based dynamic engagements with others in bridging the gap between first- and third-person approaches in understanding traumatic experiences. To put it in a provocative slogan: the second person comes first. I will conclude by briefly outlining some implications for therapeutic interventions based on dynamic body-based engagements with others complementing the static, interview-based approaches.
Citation
Ciaunica A. (2019) Subjective experiences are relational: Implications for trauma research and therapy. Constructivist Foundations 14(2): 221–224. https://constructivist.info/14/2/221
Export article citation data:
Plain Text ·
BibTex ·
EndNote ·
Reference Manager (RIS)
References
Blanke O. (2012) Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13: 556–571.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Ciaunica A. & Crucianelli L. (in press) Minimal Self-awareness from within: A developmental perspective. Journal of Consciousness Studies.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Ciaunica A. & Fotopoulou A. (2017) The touched self: Psychological and philosophical perspectives on proximal intersubjectivity and the self. In: Durt C., Fuchs T. & Tewes C. (eds.) Embodiment, enaction, and culture: Investigating the constitution of the shared world. MIT Press, Cambridge MA: 173–192.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Clark A. (2013) Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(3): 181–204.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Craig A. D. (2010) The sentient self. Brain Structure and Function 214(5–6): 563–577.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Critchley H. D., Wiens S., Rotshtein P., Öhman A. & Dolan R. D. (2004) Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience 7: 189–195.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Faivre N., Arzi A., Lunghi C. & Salomon R. (2017) Consciousness is more than meets the eye: A call for a multisensory study of subjective experience, Neuroscience of Consciousness 2017(1): nix003.
https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/nix003
Fotopoulou A. & Tsakiris M. (2017) Mentalising homeostasis: The social origins of interoceptive inference – Replies to commentaries. Neuropsychoanalysis 19(1): 71–76.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Fotopoulou K. (2015) The virtual self-mentalisation of the body as revealed in anosognosia for hemiplegia. Consciousness and Cognition 33: 500–510.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Haggard P., Taylor-Clarke M. & Kennett S. (2003) Tactile perception, cortical representation and the bodily self. Current Biology 13(5): R170-R173.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Legrand D. (2006) The bodily self: The sensorimotor roots of pre-reflective self-consciousness. Phenomenology and Cognitive Sciences 5: 89–118.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Maravita A., Spence C. & Driver J. (2003) Multisensory integration and the body schema: Close to hand and within reach. Current Biology 13(13): R531–R539.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
McGlone F., Wessberg J. & Olausson H. (2014) Discriminative and affective touch: Sensing and feeling. Neuron 82(4): 737–755.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Merleau-Ponty M. (1964) The child’s relations with others. Translated by W. Cobb. In: Edie J. M. (ed.) The primacy of perception and other essays on phenomenological psychology, the philosophy of art, history and politics. Northwestern University Press, Evanston IL: 96–155. French original published in 1960.
▸︎ Google︎ Scholar
Comments: 0
To stay informed about comments to this publication and post comments yourself, please log in first.